Are handsome people more productive?
In 1976, an
attractive young secretary to a powerful US Congressman, Wayne Hays,
acknowledged that she couldn’t type, file or answer the phone. Elizabeth Ray's skills were rather more tactile than
that.
In the past
20 years, economists have studied whether good looks lead to good wages. The answer might help explain why fashion is a
booming industry in Kazakhstan . Production of clothes in January and February
was 79% above that for the same period in 2014, according to the government’s
committee on statistics. The trick is to
find an industry in which beauty adds nothing to productivity and yet earns a
premium. Congressmen may not be the only
employers to discriminate in favor of knockouts.
The task is
not simple. Even in
industries where beauty is not a work skill, it may enable the beautiful to
produce more, because they are more self-assured. In central banking, good looks don’t directly
make inflation forecasts more accurate; but comely forecasters may have the
confidence to present their tentative results to colleagues, gathering feedback
that improves their work. We must disentangle
two effects of beauty on wages: The direct one, which reflects the employer’s
tastes; and the indirect one, which reflects the employee’s confidence.
In a 2005 experiment,
Markus Mobius and Tanya Rosenblat construct a workplace in which beauty cannot directly affect productivity – the solving of computer mazes, reminiscent of those in
white-rat studies. In the experiment, each "worker" estimates the number of mazes that she thinks
that she can complete in 15 minutes; this is a measure of her confidence. A separate panel of high school students computes a beauty score for each worker by examining her photo.
Beauts and bias
Each "employer" estimates, for each worker, the number of mazes that she can solve,
given what he knows of her, including her beauty. These estimates are used to determine the
worker’s expected wage. Employers may
learn about workers through resumes, photos, and interviews by phone or face-to-face. If beauty matters to the employer, then he
may estimate a higher productivity for a worker by examining her photo than he
would have done had he only interviewed her by phone.
The researchers
conclude that beauty raises the worker’s estimate of his own productivity – as well
as the employers’ estimates. Of the
latter, roughly 80% relate to the bosses’ own observations of beauty, and 20%
to the greater confidence of more beauteous workers.
Beauty
affects the employer’s estimate of productivity when he learns about the worker through
photos, phone interviews, and -- above all -- through face-to-face interviews. However, Mobius and Rosenblat find no
evidence that employers discriminate in favor of the handsome. They conclude this by looking at whether
beauty increases the employer’s estimate of productivity when he knows that this calculation will determine the worker's wage. Beauty
has no such effect.
In the
study, good looks affect the employer’s estimate of productivity even when he only
interviews her over the telephone. Mobius
and Rosenblat speculate that beauty may relate to “certain oral communication
skills” aside from influencing confidence. Maybe Ms. Ray should have learned how to answer the phone.
Curiously,
gender does not affect the worker’s confidence.
But men solve 30% more mazes than women do, perhaps because men are
naturally rats.
The
experiment involved 165 “employers” and 165 “employees” at La Universidad de
Tucuman in Argentina . –Leon
Taylor tayloralmaty@gmail.com
References
Markus M. Mobius and Tanya S. Rosenblat. Why beauty matters. American Economic Review 96(1): 222-35. 2006. The working paper, upon which I draw, was published by the National Bureau of Economic Research in 2005.
Wikipedia. Elizabeth Ray. Accessed March 25, 2015.
No comments:
Post a Comment